Sergiu P. Vălenaş1 & Aurora Szentágotai-Tătar2
*auraszentagotai@psychology.ro
1Evidence-Based Assessment and Psychological Interventions Doctoral School, The International Institute for the Advanced Studies of Psychotherapy and Applied Mental Health (International Institute), Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania;
2Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, The International Institute for the Advanced Studies of Psychotherapy and Applied Mental Health (International Institute), Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania;
Abstract
Over the last years, data on the relationship between rumination and executive functions has been mixed. This meta-analysis aimes to provide an estimate of the overall and specific effect sizes of the relationship between rumination and executive functions (EFs): working memory, shifting and inhibition, and to test possible moderators of these associations. From a total number of 1123 considered abstracts, 64 articles (70 studies) met our inclusion criteria. The r coefficient for every outcome measure was calculated. The overall effect size revealed no relationship between rumination and EFs (r = 0.09, p = 0.001). Also, there was no relationship between rumination and working memory (r = 0.06, p = 0.12). However, we found small effect sizes for the association between rumination and shifting (r = 0.17, p = 0.001) and between rumination and inhibition (r = 0.11, p = 0.012). The only significant moderators were type of sample and sample size, which moderated the rumination – inhibition relation. These findings suggest that rumination may be associated with some EFs (i.e. inhibition, shifting) but the magnitude of this association is relatively small. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Keywords: Rumination, Executive Functions, Inhibition, Shifting, Working Memory
Published online: 2017/09/01
Published print: 2017/09/01
References
References marked with an asterisk (*) denote articles included in the meta-analysis.
*Aker, M., Harmer, C., & Landrø, N. I. (2014). More rumination and less effective emotion regulation in previously depressed women with preserved executive functions. BMC Psychiatry, 14. doi: 10.1186/s12888-014-0334-4
*Alderman, B. L., Olson, R. L., Bates, M. E., Selby, E. A., Buckman, J. F., Brush, C. J., … Shors, T. J. (2015). Rumination in major depressive disorder is associated with impaired neural activation during conflict monitoring. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 269–269. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00269
*Altamirano, L. J., Miyake, A., & Whitmer, A. J. (2010). When mental inflexibility facilitates executive control: Beneficial side effects of ruminative tendencies on goal maintenance. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1377–1382. doi:10.1177/0956797610381505
Anderson, P. J. (2008). Towards a developmental model of executive function. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, & P. J. Anderson (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 3–21). Philadelphia, PA, US: Taylor & Francis.
Baert, S., Koster, E. H., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Modification of information-processing biases in emotional disorders: Clinically relevant developments in experimental psychopathology. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 4(2), 208–222. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2011.4.2.208
Baldwin, S., & Shadish, W. (2011). A Primer on Meta-analysis in Clinical Psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 2(2), 294–317. doi: 10.5127/jep.009610
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. University of Pennsylvania Press.
*Beckwé, M., Deroost, N., Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2014). Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychological Research, 78(5), 651–660. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0517-5
*Bernblum, R., & Mor, N. (2010). Rumination and emotion-related biases in refreshing information. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10(3), 423–432. doi: 10.1037/a0018427
*Bistricky, S. L. (2012). Inhibitory processing of sad facial expressions and depression vulnerability.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Front Matter. In Introduction to Meta-Analysis(pp. i-xxix). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386.fmatter/summary
Brozovich, F., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). An analysis of post-event processing in social anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(6), 891–903. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.01.002
*Connolly, S. L., Wagner, C. A., Shapero, B. G., Pendergast, L. L., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2014). Rumination prospectively predicts executive functioning impairments in adolescents. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(1), 46–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.07.009
*Cox, R. C., Ebesutani, C., & Olatunji, B. O. (2015). Linking sleep disturbance and maladaptive repetitive thought: The role of executive function. Cognitive Therapy and Research. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9713-5
Cristea, I. A., Matu, S., Szentagotai Tatar, A., & David, D. (2013). The other side of rumination: reflective pondering as a strategy for regulating emotions in social situations. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 26(5), 584–594. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2012.725469
*Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Soleti, E., & Rimé, B. (2013). Negative emotional experiences arouse rumination and affect working memory capacity. Emotion, 13(5), 867–880. doi: 10.1037/a0032492
*Curci, A., Soleti, E., Lanciano, T., Doria, V., Rimé, B., & Rime, B. (2015). Balancing emotional processing with ongoing cognitive activity: the effects of task modality on intrusions and rumination. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(August), 1275–1275. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01275
*Daches, S., & Mor, N. (2014). Training ruminators to inhibit negative information: A preliminary report. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(2), 160–171. doi: 10.1007/s10608-013-9585-5
*Daches, S., Mor, N., & Hertel, P. (2015). Rumination: Cognitive consequences of training to inhibit the negative. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 49, 76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.01.010
*Daches, S., Mor, N., Winquist, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2010). Brooding and attentional control in processing self-encoded information: Evidence from a modified Garner task. Cognition & Emotion, 24(5), 876–885. doi: 10.1080/02699930902954825
*Dai, Q., Rahman, S., Lau, B., Sook Kim, H., Deldin, P., Kim, H. S., & Deldin, P. (2015). The influence of self-relevant materials on working memory in dysphoric undergraduates. Psychiatry Research, 229(3), 858–866. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.068
*Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(6), 699–711. doi: 10.1023/A:1005591412406
*Delespaux, E., & Zech, E. (2015). Why Do Bereaved Partners Experience Interfering Rumination?: Evidence for Deficits in Cognitive Inhibition. Death studies, 39(8), 463-472. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2014.958631
*De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2011). Depressive symptoms and cognitive control in a mixed antisaccade task: Specific effects of depressive rumination. Cognition and Emotion, 25(5), 886–897. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.514711
*De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Emotional interference in working memory is related to rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(4), 348–357. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9352-4
*De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2010). The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 264–280. doi: 10.1080/02699930903378354
*De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Everaert, J., Schacht, R., Van den Abeele, D., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Internal cognitive control in clinical depression: General but no emotion-specific impairments. Psychiatry Research, 199(2), 124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.019
*Demeyer, I., De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Rumination mediates the relationship between impaired cognitive control for emotional information and depressive symptoms: A prospective study in remitted depressed adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(5), 292–297. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.02.012
*Ding, X., Yang, Y., Qian, M., & Gordon-Hollingsworth, A. (2015). SPECIFIC EFFECTS OF ANGER RUMINATION ON PARTICULAR EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS (.). Psychological Reports, 117(3), 825–841. doi: 10.2466/02.09.PR0.117c24z8
Donaldson, C. (2005). Depressive rumination: Nature, theory and treatment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(1), 140–141. doi: 10.1348/014466505X103762
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56(2), 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x
*Exner, C., Martin, V., & Rief, W. (2009). Self-focused ruminations and memory deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33(2), 163–174. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9162-x
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(1), 101–135. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Defries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 137(2), 201–225. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201
*Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Baert, S., & Koster, E. H. W. (2006). Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93(1-3), 149–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.007
Hertel, P. T., & Rude, S. S. (1991). Depressive deficits in memory: focusing attention improves subsequent recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 120(3), 301–309. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.3.301
Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21(11), 1539–1558. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186
*Hoorelbeke, K., Koster, E. H. W. W., Vanderhasselt, M.-A., Callewaert, S., & Demeyer, I. (2015). The influence of cognitive control training on stress reactivity and rumination in response to a lab stressor and naturalistic stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 69, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.03.010
*Hsu, K. J., Beard, C., Rifkin, L., Dillon, D. G., Pizzagalli, D. A., & Björgvinsson, T. (2015). Transdiagnostic mechanisms in depression and anxiety: The role of rumination and attentional control. Journal of Affective Disorders, 188, 22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.008
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(19), 6829–6833. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801268105
*Joormann, J. (2006). Differential Effects of Rumination and Dysphoria on the Inhibition of Irrelevant Emotional Material: Evidence from a Negative Priming Task. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 30(2), 149–160. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9035-8
Joormann, J. (2010). Cognitive inhibition and emotion regulation in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 161–166. doi: 10.1177/0963721410370293
*Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 182–192. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.182
*Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 281–298. doi: 10.1080/02699930903407948
*Joormann, J., Nee, D. E., Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Interference Resolution in Major Depression, 10(1), 21–33. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.1.21.Interference
Joormann, J., Yoon, K. L., & Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 128–139. doi: 10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.002
Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z
Karbach, J., & Unger, K. (2014). Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 390. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00390
*Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Rumination is characterized by valence-specific impairments in switching of attention. Acta Psychologica, 144(3), 563–570. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.09.008
Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005
Kuehner, C., Huffziger, S., & Liebsch, K. (2009). Rumination, distraction and mindful self-focus: Effects on mood, dysfunctional attitudes and cortisol stress response. Psychological Medicine, 39(2), 219. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003553
*Lau, M. A., Christensen, B. K., Hawley, L. L., Gemar, M. S., & Segal, Z. V. (2007). Inhibitory deficits for negative information in persons with major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 37(9), 1249–1259. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000530
*Letkiewicz, A. M., Miller, G. A., Crocker, L. D., Warren, S. L., Infantolino, Z. P., Mimnaugh, K. J., & Heller, W. (2014). Executive function deficits in daily life prospectively predict increases in depressive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(6), 612–620. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9629-5
*Levens, S. M., Muhtadie, L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Rumination and impaired resource allocation in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(4), 757–766. doi: 10.1037/a0017206
MacLeod, C. M. (2007). The concept of inhibition in cognition. In D. S. Gorfein & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Inhibition in cognition (pp. 3–23). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107–123. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.111.1.107
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1–47). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270–291. doi: 10.1037/a0028228
*Miranda, R., Valderrama, J., Tsypes, A., Gadol, E., & Gallagher, M. (2013). Cognitive inflexibility and suicidal ideation: Mediating role of brooding and hopelessness. Psychiatry Research, 210(1), 174–181. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.02.033
*Mitchell, L., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2012). Relationships between insomnia, negative emotionality and attention control. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 10(3), 237–243. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2012.00567.x
Miyake, A. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
Mor, N., & Daches, S. (2015). Ruminative thinking lessons learned from cognitive training. Clinical Psychological Science, 2167702615578130. doi: 10.1177/2167702615578130
Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(3), 519. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.3.519
Moulds, M. L., Kandris, E., Starr, S., & Wong, A. C. M. (2007). The relationship between rumination, avoidance and depression in a non-clinical sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(2), 251–261. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.003
Nolan, S. A., Roberts, J. E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Neuroticism and ruminative response style as predictors of change in depressive symptomatology. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22(5), 445–455. doi:10.1023/A:1018769531641
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 504. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition & Emotion, 7(6), 561–570. doi:10.1080/02699939308409206
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 198. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking Rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, \i3(5), 400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
*Onraedt, T., & Koster, E. H. W. (2014). Training working memory to reduce rumination. PLoS ONE, 9(3). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0090632
Orwin, R. G. (1983). A Fail-SafeN for Effect Size in Meta-Analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 8(2), 157–159. doi:10.3102/10769986008002157
*Owens, M., & Derakshan, N. (2013). The effects of dysphoria and rumination on cognitive flexibility and task selection. Acta Psychologica, 142(3), 323–331. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.01.008
Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2003). An empirical test of a clinical metacognitive model of rumination and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 261–273. doi:10.1023/A:1023962332399
*Pe, M. L., Koval, P., Houben, M., Erbas, Y., Champagne, D., & Kuppens, P. (2015). Updating in working memory predicts greater emotion reactivity to and facilitated recovery from negative emotion-eliciting stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(MAR). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00372
*Pe, M. L., Raes, F., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The cognitive building blocks of emotion regulation: Ability to update working memory moderates the efficacy of rumination and reappraisal on emotion. PLoS ONE, 8(7). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069071
*Philippot, P., & Brutoux, F. (2008). Induced rumination dampens executive processes in dysphoric young adults. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(3), 219–227. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.07.001
*Polanco-Roman, L., Jurska, J., Quiñones, V., Miranda, R., Quinones, V., & Miranda, R. (2015). Brooding, Reflection, and Distraction: Relation to Non-Suicidal Self-Injury versus Suicide Attempts. Archives of Suicide Research : Official Journal of the International Academy for Suicide Research, 19(3), 350–365. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2014.981623
*Quinn, M. E., & Joormann, J. (2015). Stress-induced changes in executive control are associated with depression symptoms: Examining the role of rumination. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 628–636. doi: 10.1177/2167702614563930
*Quinn, M. E., Keil, D. C., Utke, S., & Joormann, J. (2014). Trait rumination moderates the effect of executive control training. [References]. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 5(3), 289–301. doi: 10.5127/jep.0038713
*Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., Demyttenaere, K., Sabbe, B., Pieters, G., & Eelen, P. (2006). Is overgeneral autobiographical memory an isolated memory phenomenon in major depression? Memory, 14(5), 584–594. doi: 10.1080/09658210600624614
*Robinaugh, D. J., Crane, M. E., Enock, P. M., & McNally, R. J. (2015). Training the removal of negative information from working memory: A preliminary investigation of a working memory bias modification task. Cognition & Emotion, (March 2015), 1–12. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1014312
*Schmalzried, R. C. (2013). Patterns of rumination by young and older adults., 73(12-B(E)), No Pagination Specified.
*Segerstrom, S. C., Roach, A. R., Evans, D. R., Schipper, L. J., & Darville, A. K. (2010). The structure and health correlates of trait repetitive thought in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 25(3), 505–15. doi: 10.1037/a0019456
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628–654. doi: 10.1037/a0027473
Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., & Thase, M. E. (2007). Neurobehavioral therapies in the 21st century: Summary of an emerging field and an extended example of cognitive control training for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31(2), 235–262. doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9118-6
*Smith, L. M. (2013). Rumination , Negative Affect and Working Memory : Does Rumination Moderate the Relationship between Negative Affect Induction and Working Memory ? Lauren M . Smith A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Docto.
*Sumner, J. A., Griffith, J. W., & Mineka, S. (2011). Examining the mechanisms of overgeneral autobiographical memory: Capture and rumination, and impaired executive control. Memory, 19(2), 169–183. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2010.541467
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259. doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561
Vălenaș, S., & Szentagotai, A. (2014). Rumination in Generalized Anxiety and Social Phobia: A Qualitative Systematic Review. Transylvanian Journal of Psychology, 15(1), 61–88.
*Vanderhasselt, M.-A., Baeken, C., Van Schuerbeek, P., Luypaert, R., De Mey, J., & De Raedt, R. (2013). How brooding minds inhibit negative material: An event-related fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 81(3), 352–359. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.01.007
*Vanderhasselt, M.-A., Brunoni, A. R., Loeys, T., Boggio, P. S., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Nosce te ipsum—Socrates revisited? Controlling momentary ruminative self-referent thoughts by neuromodulation of emotional working memory. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2581–2589. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.011
*Vanderhasselt, M.-A., De Raedt, R., Namur, V., Lotufo, P. A., Bensenor, I. M., Boggio, P. S., … Isabela M. Bensenor3, P. S. B., Andre R. Brunoni. (2015). Transcranial electric stimulation and neurocognitive training in clinically depressed patients: a pilot study of the effects on rumination. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 57(9), 93–99. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.09.015
*Vergara-Lopez, C., Lopez-Vergara, H. I., & Roberts, J. E. (2016). Testing a “content meets process” model of depression vulnerability and rumination: Exploring the moderating role of set-shifting deficits. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 50, 201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.08.002
*von Hippel, W., Vasey, M. W., Gonda, T., & Stern, T. (2008). Executive function deficits, rumination and late-onset depressive symptoms in older adults. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(4), 474–487. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9034-9
Wagner, C. A., Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (2015). Trait rumination, depression, and executive functions in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(1), 18–36. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0133-8
*Wanmaker, S., Geraerts, E., & Franken, I. H. A. (2015). A working memory training to decrease rumination in depressed and anxious individuals: A double-blind randomized controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175C. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.027
Ward, A., Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Can’t Quite Commit: Rumination and Uncertainty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(1), 96–107. doi: 10.1177/0146167202238375
*Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 72(3), 400–402. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.72.3.400
Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), 353. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.333
Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82(1), 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.10.006
*Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition Versus Switching Deficits in Different Forms of Rumination. Psychological Science, 18(6), 546–553. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01936.x
*Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2010). Trait rumination and inhibitory deficits in long-term memory. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 168–179. doi:10.1080/02699930802645762
*Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Switching and backward inhibition in major depressive disorder: The role of rumination. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(3), 570–578. doi:10.1037/a0027474
Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1036–1061. doi:10.1037/a0030923
*Wilkinson, P. O., & Goodyer, I. M. (2006). Attention difficulties and mood-related ruminative response style in adolescents with unipolar depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 47(12), 1284–1291. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01660.x
*Yoon, K. L., Lemoult, J., & Joormann, J. (2014). Updating emotional content in working memory: A depression-specific deficit? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(3), 368–374. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.03.004
*Zetsche, U., D’Avanzato, C., & Joormann, J. (2012). Depression and rumination: Relation to components of inhibition. Cognition and Emotion, 26(4), 758–767. doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.613919
*Zetsche, U., & Joormann, J. (2011). Components of interference control predict depressive symptoms and rumination cross-sectionally and at six months follow-up. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(1). doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.06.001
Acknowledgment
This work was supported by a grant of the Romanian Ministry of Education and Scientific Research and Executive Agency for Higher Education, Research, Development and Innovation, MECS–UEFISCDI, PN II–PT–PCCA–2013–4–1797.